Kognition und Wahrnehmung

Kognition und Wahrnehmung

Publikationen

Copyright Notice: This material is presented to ensure timely dissemination of scholarly work. Copyright and all rights therein are retained by the authors or by other copyright holders. All persons copying this information are expected to adhere to the terms and constraints invoked by each author's copyright. In most cases, this material may not be reposted without the explicit permission of the copyright holder.

Note: Because of copyright restrictions, the linked PDFs are password-protected. William "JAMES" might help you to open locked doors.


2024

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Ulrich, R. & Kaup, B. (2023). Association between abstraction level and time: Are future and past more abstract than the present? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 1595-1609. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218231217732
  • Bratzke, D., Peris, L. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Time and visual-spatial illusions: Evidence for cross-dimensional interference between duration and illusory size. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 86, 567-578. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02737-x
  • Ellinghaus, R., Bausenhart, K. M., Koc, D., Ulrich, R. & Liepelt, R. (2024). Order effects in stimulus discrimination challenge established models of comparative judgement: A meta-analytic review of the Type B effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02479-3
  • Iberl, B., Aljovic, A., Ulrich, R. & Reiber, F. (2024). The Poisson extension of the unrelated question model: Improving survey statistics with time-constrained questions on sensitive topics. Survey Research Methods, 18, 21-38. https://doi.org/10.18148/srm/2024.v18i1.8252
  • Johansson, R. C. G., Kelber, P. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Speeded classification of visual events is sensitive to crossmodal intensity correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 50, 554–569. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001183
  • Johansson, R. C. G. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Serial processing of proximity groups and similarity groups. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 86, 1303-1317. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-024-02861-2
  • Kaup, B., Ulrich, R., Bausenhart, K. M., Bryce, D., Butz, M. V., Dignath, D., Dudschig, C., Franz, V. H., Friedrich, C., Gawrilow, C., Heller, J., Huff, M., Hütter, M., Janczyk, M., Leuthold, H., Mallot, H., Nürk, H.-C., Ramscar, M., Said, N., Svaldi, J. & Wong, H. Y. (2024). Modal and amodal cognition: An overarching principle in various domains of psychology. Psychological Research, 88, 307-337. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01878-w
  • Kelber, P., Mackenzie, I. G. & Mittelstädt, V. (2024). Cognitive control in cross-modal contexts: Abstract feature transitions of task-related but not task-unrelated stimuli modulate the congruency sequence effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 50, 902-919. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001300
  • Kelber, P., Mackenzie, I. G. & Mittelstädt, V. (2024). Transfer of cognitive control adjustments within and between speakers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218241249471
  • Kelber, P., Mittelstädt, V. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Interplay of aging and practice in conflict processing: A big-data diffusion-model analysis. Psychology and Aging. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000848
  • Kelber, P. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Independent-channels models of temporal-order judgment revisited: A model comparison. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 86, 2187-2209. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-024-02915-5
  • Koob, V., Sauerbier, C., Schröter, H., Ulrich, R. & Janczyk, M. (2024). Separating facilitation and interference in backward crosstalk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 50, 295-312. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001184
  • Reiber, F., Bryce, D. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Self-protecting responses in randomized response designs: A survey on intimate partner violence during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic. Sociological Methods & Research, 53, 296-327. https://doi.org/10.1177/00491241211043138
  • Reiber, F. & Ulrich, R. (2024). Exploring effects of age on conflict processing in the light of practice in a large-scale dataset. Experimental Aging Research, 50, 422-442. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2023.2214051
  • Smith, P. R. W., & Ulrich, R. (2024). The neutral condition in conflict tasks: On the violation of the midpoint assumption in reaction time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 1023-1043. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218231201476
  • Ulrich, R., de la Vega, I., Eikmeier, V., Günther, F., & Kaup, B. (2024). Mental association of time and valence. Memory & Cognition, 52, 444-458. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-023-01473-9
  • Ulrich, R. & Kaup, B. (2024). Wilhelm Wundt: His bumpy start in science at the University of Tübingen. History of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/hop0000251

2023

  • Eberhart, J., Koepp, A. E., Howard, S. J., Kok, R., McCoy, D. C., & Baker, S. T. (2023). Advancing educational research on children’s self-regulation with observational measures. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 41, 267-282. https://doi.org/10.1177/07342829221143208
  • Eberhart, J., Paes, T. M., Ellefson, M. R., & Marcovitch, S. (2023). Special issue editorial: Executive functions and play. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 30, Article 100198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100198
  • Iberl, B., & Ulrich, R. (2023). On estimating the frequency of a target behavior from time-constrained yes/no survey questions: A parametric approach based on the Poisson process. Psychological Methods. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000588
  • Janczyk, M., Koch, I., & Ulrich, R. (2023). Is there a cognitive link between the domains of deictic time and number? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 49, 493–507. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001162
  • Kelber, P., Gierlich, M., Göth, J., Jeschke, M. G., Mackenzie, I. G., & Mittelstädt, V. (2023). A diffusion model analysis of object-based selective attention in the Eriksen flanker task. Experimental Psychology, 70, 155-170. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000588
  • Koob, V., Mackenzie, I., Ulrich, R., Leuthold, H., & Janczyk, M. (2023). The role of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information in congruency sequence effects: Applying the diffusion model for conflict tasks. Cognitive Psychology140, Article 101528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101528
  • Koob, V., Ulrich, R., & Janczyk, M. (2023). Response activation and activation–transmission in response-based backward crosstalk: Analyses and simulations with an extended diffusion model. Psychological Review, 130, 102–136. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000326
  • Mittelstädt, V., Ulrich, R., König, J., Hofbauer, K., & Mackenzie, I. G. (2023). The influence of reward in the Simon task: Differences and similarities to the Stroop and Eriksen flanker tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 85, 949-959. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-022-02563-7
  • Potamianou, H., & Bryce, D. (2023). How flexible is cognitive control? (Mouse) tracking conflict adaptation across context similarities. Psychological Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01874-0
  • Reiber, F., Pope, H. G., & Ulrich, R. (2023). Cheater detection using the unrelated question model. Sociological Methods & Research52, 389-411. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124120914919
  • Richter, T., Ulrich, R., & Janczyk, M. (2023). Diffusion models with time-dependent parameters: An analysis of computational effort and accuracy of different numerical methods. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 114, Article 102756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2023.102756
  • Schlotterbeck, F., Augurzky, P. & Ulrich, R. (2023). Degree of incrementality is modulated by experimental context – ERP evidence from German quantifier restriction. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 38, 390-410. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2022.2125541
  • Schonard, C., Ulrich, R., & Janczyk, M. (2023). Temporal aspects of two types of backward crosstalk in dual-tasks: An analysis of continuous mouse-tracking data. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology76, 2068-2083. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218221135603
  • Ulrich, R., Bausenhart, K. M., & Wearden, J. H. (2023). Weber’s law for timing and time perception: Reconciling the Poisson clock with scalar expectancy theory (SET). Timing & Time Perception11, 167-197. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10055
  • Ulrich, R., Cléret, L., Comstock, R. D., Kanayama, G., Simon, P., & Pope, H. G. (2023). Assessing the prevalence of doping among elite athletes: An analysis of results generated by the single sample count method versus the unrelated question method. Sports Medicine - Open, 9, Article 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40798-023-00658-5

2022

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Ulrich, R., & Kaup, B. (2022). Association between abstraction level and time: Are future and past more abstract than the present? OSF preprints. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/a7zsx
  • Bratzke, D., Bryce, D. (2022). Subjective estimates of total processing time in dual-tasking: (some) good news for bad introspection. Psychological Research, 87, 1560-1568. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01762-z
  • Bratzke, D., & Bryce, D. (2022). Timing of internal processes: Investigating introspection about the costs of task switching and memory search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 84, 1501–1508. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-022-02510-6
  • Bryce, D., & Bratzke, D. (2022). The surprising role of stimulus modality in the dual-task introspective blind spot: A memory account. Psychological Research86, 1332-1354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01545-y
  • Kattner, F., & Bryce, D. (2022). Attentional control and metacognitive monitoring of the effects of different types of task-irrelevant sound on serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48, 139–158. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000982
  • Kaup, B., Ulrich, R., Bausenhart, K. M., Bryce, D., et al. (2022). Modal and amodal cognition: An overarching principle in various domains of psychology. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/fz85n
  • Mackenzie, I. G., Mittelstädt, V., Ulrich, R., & Leuthold, H. (2022). The role of temporal order of relevant and irrelevant dimensions within conflict tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48, 1099–1115. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001032
  • Mahani, M.-A. N., Bausenhart, K. M., Ulrich, R., & Ahmadabadi, M. N. (2022). Learning to integrate an artificial sensory device: How Bayesian integration may lead to nonoptimal perception. IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems, 14, 1755-1765. https://doi.org/10.1109/TCDS.2022.3144092
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2022). Optimizing research output: How can psychological research methods be improved? Annual Review of Psychology, 73, 691–718. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-094927
  • Mittelstädt, V., Miller, J., Leuthold, H., Mackenzie, I. G., & Ulrich, R. (2022). The time-course of distractor-based activation modulates effects of speed-accuracy tradeoffs in conflict tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29, 837–854. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-02003-x
  • Reiber, F., Bryce, D., & Ulrich, R. (2022). Self-protecting responses in randomized response designs: A survey on intimate partner violence during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sociological Methods and Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/00491241211043138
  • Reiber, F., Schnuerch, M., & Ulrich, R. (2022). Improving the efficiency of surveys with randomized response models: A sequential approach based on curtailed sampling. Psychological Methods, 27, 198–211. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000353
  • Seiffer, B., Hautzinger, M., Ulrich, R., & Wolf, S. (2022). The efficacy of physical activity for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Journal of Attention Disorders26, 656–673. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547211017982
  • Winter, A., Dudschig, C., Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Kaup, B. (2022). The action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE): Meta-analysis of a benchmark finding for embodiment. Acta Psychologica, 230, Article 103712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103712

2021

  • Baker, S. T., Le Courtois, S., & Eberhart, J. (2021). Making space for children’s agency with playful learning. International Journal of Early Years Education, 31, 372–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2021.1997726
  • Bausenhart, K. M., Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2021). Effects of conflict trial proportion: A comparison of the Eriksen and Simon tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics83, 810–836. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02164-2
  • Bratzke, D., Janczyk, M. (2021). Introspection about backward crosstalk in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 85, 605–617. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01282-3
  • Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2021). Mental imagery of free fall: Does a falling apple accelerate in our minds? Timing & Time Perception9, 150-160. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10022
  • Ellinghaus, R., Giel, S., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2021). Humans integrate duration information across sensory modalities: Evidence for an amodal internal reference of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47, 1205-1225. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001002
  • Kaup, B., Scherer, N., & Ulrich, R. (2021). Associations between abstract concepts: Investigating the relationship between deictic time and valence. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 612720. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.612720
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2021). A simple, general, and efficient method for sequential hypothesis testing: The independent segments procedure. Psychological Methods, 26, 486–497. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000350
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2021). Alternative sequential methods in statistical testing: A reply to Lakens (2021) and Erdfelder and Schnuerch (2021). Psychological Methods, 26, 2021, 507-512. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000420
  • Ulrich, R., Prislan, L., & Miller, J. (2021). A bimodal extension of the Eriksen flanker task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 83, 790–799. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02150-8
  • von Sobbe, L., Maienborn, C., Reiber, F., Scheifele, E., & Ulrich, R. (2021) Speed or duration? Effects of implicit stimulus attributes on perceived duration, Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 33, 877-898. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2021.1950736
  • von Sobbe, L., Ulrich, R., Gangloff, L., Scheifele, E. & Maienborn, C. (2021). Is rushing always faster than strolling? A reaction time study on the processing of sentences containing manner of motion verbs. Acta Psychologica221, Article 103428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103428

2020

  • Augurzky, P., Hohaus, V., & Ulrich, R. (2020). Context and complexity in incremental sentence interpretation: An ERP study on temporal quantification. Cognitive Science, 44, Article e12913. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12913
  • Augurzky, P., Schlotterbeck, F., & Ulrich, R. (2020). Most (but not all) quantifiers are interpreted immediately in visual context. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35, 1203–1222. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2020.1722846
  • Bratzke, D. & Ulrich, R. (2020). Short-term memory of temporal information revisited. Psychological Research, 85, 1776-1782. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01343-y
  • Colling, L. J., Szűcs, D., De Marco, D., Cipora, K., Ulrich, R., Nuerk, H.-C., … McShane, B. B. (2020). Registered replication report on Fischer, Castel, Dodd, and Pratt (2003). Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science3, 143-162. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245920903079
  • Heller, S., Ulrich, R., Perikles, S., & Dietz, P. (2020). Refined analysis of a cross-sectional doping survey among recreational triathletes: Support for the nutritional supplement gateway hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 561013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.561013
  • Koob, V., Durst, M., Bratzke, D., Ulrich, R., & Janczyk, M. (2020). S1-R2 and R1-R2 backward crosstalk both affect the central processing stage. Journal of Cognition, 3, Article 37. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.121
  • Schonard, C., Ulrich, R., & Janczyk, M. (2020). The backward crosstalk effect does not depend on the degree of a preceding response conflict. Experimental Psychology, 67, 277–291. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000498
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2020). Meta-research: Questionable research practices may have little effect on replicability. eLife, 9, Article e58237. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.58237

2019

  • Augurzky, P., Franke, M, & Ulrich, R. (2019). Gricean expectations in online sentence comprehension: an ERP study on the processing of scalar inferences. Cognitive Science, 43, e12776
  • Birngruber, T. & Ulrich, R. (2019). Perceived duration increases not only with physical but also with implicit size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45, 969-979.
  • Bratzke, D., & Bryce, D. (2019). Introspection is not always blind to the costs of multitasking: The case of task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45, 980-992.
  • Bratzke, D. & Ulrich, R. (2019a). Temporal reproduction within and across senses: Testing the supramodal property of the Pacemaker-Counter model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 1218-1235.
  • Bratzke, D & Ulrich, R. (2019b). Temporal sequence discrimination within and across senses: Do we really hear what we see? Experimental Brain Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-019-05654-4
  • Durst, M., Ulrich, R., & Janczyk, M. (2019). To prepare or not to prepare? When preparation of a response in Task 2 induces extra performance costs in Task 1. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 654-660.
  • Ellinghaus, R., Gick, M., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2019). Decay of internal reference information in duration discrimination: Intertrial interval modulates the Type B effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1578–1586. doi: 10.1177/1747021818808187
  • Janczyk, M. & Ulrich, R. (2019).  Action consequences affect the space-time congruency effect on reaction time.  Acta Psychologica, 198
  • Mahani, M.-A. N., Bausenhart, K. M., Ahmadabadi, M. N., & Ulrich, R. (2019). Multimodal Simon effect: A multimodal extension of the Diffusion Model for Conflict Tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 1–17.
  • Miller, J. & Ulrich, R. (2019). The quest for an optimal alpha. Plos One, 14(1), e0208631.
  • Pincham, H. L., Bryce, D., Fonagy, P., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2019). Psychosocial intervention in at-risk adolescents: using event-related potentials to assess changes in decision making and feedback processing. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 2, 223-236. ()
  • Ulrich, R. & Bausenhart, K. M. (2019). The Temporal Oddball Effect and related phenomena: Cognitive mechanisms and experimental approaches. In V. Arstila, A. Bardon, S.E. Power, & A. Vatakis (Eds.): The Illusions of Time: Philosophical and Psychological Essays on Timing and Time Perception (pp. 71-89). Palgrave Macmillan, Cham.
  • von Sobbe, L., Scheifele, E., Maienborn, C., & Ulrich, R. (2019). The space – time congruency effect: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Science, 43, 1–23.

2018

  • Bausenhart, K.M., Di Luca, M., & Ulrich, R. (2018). Assessing duration discrimination: Psychophysical methods and psychometric function analysis. In A. Vatakis, F. Balci, M. Di Luca & A. Correa (Eds.), Timing and Time Perception: Procedures, Measures, and Applications (pp. 52–78). Leiden: Brill
  • Bausenhart, K. M., & Quinn, K. R. (2018). On the interplay of visuospatial and audiotemporal dominance: Evidence from a multimodal Kappa effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 535-552.
  • Birngruber, T., Schröter, H., Schütt, E., & Ulrich, R. (2018). Stimulus expectation prolongs rather than shortens perceived duration: Evidence from self-generated expectations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44, 117-127.
  • Birngruber, T. & Ulrich, R. (2018). Perceived duration increases not only with physical but also with implicit size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
  • Bratzke, D., & Bryce, D. (2018). Introspection is not always blind to the costs of multitasking: The case of task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000635
  • Dietz, P., Iberl, B., Schuett, E., Poppel, M. Van, Ulrich, R., & Sattler, M. C. (2018). Prevalence Estimates for Pharmacological Neuroenhancement in Austrian University Students: Its Relation to Health-Related Risk Attitude and the Framing Effect of Caffeine Tablets. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9:494. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2018.00494
  • Dietz, P., Quermann, A., van Poppel, M.N.M., Striegel, H., Schröter, H., Ulrich R., & Simon, P. (2018). Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model (UQM): Assessing the influence of the probability of receiving the sensitive question on prevalence estimation. PLoS ONE 13(5): e0197270.
  • Ellinghaus, R., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2018). Effects of stimulus order on comparative judgments across stimulus attributes and sensory modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(1), 7-12. ()
  • Erdfelder, E., & Ulrich, R. (2018). Zur Methodologie von Replikationsstudien. Psychologische Rundschau. 69, 3-21.
  • Scheifele, E., Eikmeier, V., Alex-Ruf, S., Maienborn, C., & Ulrich, R. (2018). A replication of “Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses (Sell & Kaschak, 2011; Experiment 1)”. The Quantiative Methods for Psychology, 14, 8–11. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.14.1.r008
  • Ulrich, R., Miller, J., & Erdfelder, E. (2018). Effect size estimation from t-statistics in the presence of publication bias: A brief review of existing approaches with some extensions. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 226, 56-80.
  • Ulrich, R., Pope, H. G., Cléret, L., Petróczi, A., Nepusz, T., Schaffer, J., Kanayama, G, Comstock, R. D., & Simon, P. (2018). Doping in two elite athletics competitions assessed by randomized-response surveys. Sports Medicine, 48, 211-219.

2017

  • Bratzke, D., Birngruber, T., Durst, M., & Schröter, H. (2017). Filled and empty reproductions of filled and empty intervals: Is there also a filled reproduction illusion? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79, 2143-2152.
  • Bryce, D., & Bratzke, D. (2017). Are participants' reports of their own reaction times reliable? Re-examining introspective limitations in active and passive dual-task paradigms. Acta Psychologica, 172, 1-9. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.10.00 7()
  • Mahani, M.-A. N., Sheybani, S., Bausenhart, K. M., Ulrich, R., & Ahmadabadi, M. N. (2017). Multisensory perception of contradictory information in an environment of varying reliability: Evidence for conscious perception and optimal causal inference. Scientific Reports, 7:3167. ()
  • Bott, O., Augurzky, P., Sternefeld, W., & Ulrich, R. (2017). Incremental generation of answers during the comprehension of questions with quantifiers. Cognition, 166, 328-343.
  • Ellinghaus, R., Karlbauer, M., Bausenhart, K.M., & Ulrich, R. (2017). On the time course of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Psychological Research. ()
  • Ellinghaus, R., & Miller, J. (2017). Delta plots with negative-going slopes as a potential marker of decreasing response activation in masked semantic priming. Psychological Research. doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0844-z
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2017). Some properties of p-curves, with an application to gradual response bias. Psychological Methods. dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000125
  • Kaup, B., & Ulrich, R. (2017). Die Beziehung zwischen sprachlicher und nicht-sprachlicher Kognition: Die Bedeutung von Repräsentationsformaten. Psychologische Rundschau, 68, 115-130
  • de la Vega, I., Eikmeier, V., Ulrich, R., & Kaup, B. (2017). The mental timeline in a crossed-hands paradigm: A matter of instruction. Experimental Psychology, 63, 326-332.

2016

  • Augurzky, P., Bott,O, Sternefeld, W., & Ulrich, R. (2016). Are all the triangles blue? ERP evidence for the incremental processing of German quantifier restriction. Language and Cognition. 1-34. doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2016.30
  • Bausenhart, K. M., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2016). Formation and representation of temporal reference information. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 8, 46-52. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.01.007. ()
  • Bermeitinger, C., Kaup, B., Kiesel, A., Koch, I., Kunde, W., Müsseler, J., … Ulrich, R. (2016). Positionspapier zur Lage der Allgemeinen Psychologie. Psychologische Rundschau, 67, 175-207. ()
  • Bratzke, D., & Bryce, D. (2016). Temporal discrimination of one’s own reaction times in dual-task performance: Context effects and methodological constraints. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 1806-1816. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1161-0 ()
  • Bratzke, D., Quinn, K. R., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2016). Representations of temporal information in short-term memory: Are they modality-specific? Acta Psychologica, 170, 163-167. ()
  • Bryce, D., & Bratzke, D. (2016). Multiple timing of nested intervals: Further evidence for a weighted sum of segments account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 317-323. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0877-5 ()
  • De la Vega, I., Eikmeier, V., Ulrich, R., & Kaup, B. (2016). The mental timeline in a crossed-hands paradigm: A matter of instruction. Experimental Psychology, 63, 326-332. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000342 ()
  • Dietz, P., Dalaker,R., Letzel, S., Ulrich, R. & Simon, P. (2016). Analgesics use in competitive triathletes: its relationship to doping and on predicting its usage. Journal of Sports Sciences, doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1149214 ()
  • Eikmeier, V., Alex-Ruf, S., Maienborn, C., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2016). The mental timeline during the processing of linguistic information. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualizations of Time (pp. 103–122). Amsterdam, Holland: John Benjamins Publishing Co.
  • Janczyk, M., Durst, M., & Ulrich, R. (2016). Action selection by temporally distal goal states. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 67, 1-7. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2016). Interpreting confidence intervals: A comment on Hoekstra, Morey, Rouder, and Wagenmakers (2014). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 124–130. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2016). Optimizing research payoff. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11,664-691.
  • Pincham, H. L., Bryce, D., Kokorikou, D., Fonagy, P., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2016). Psychosocial intervention is associated with altered emotion processing: An event-related potential study in at-risk adolescents. PLOS One, 11(1), e0147357. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147357 ()
  • Schröter, H., Studzinski, B., Dietz, P., Ulrich, R., Striegel, H., & Simon, P. (2016). A comparison of the cheater detection and the unrelated question models: A randomized response survey on physical and cognitive doping in recreational triathletes. Plos One, 11(5), e0155765. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Erdfelder, E., Deutsch, R., Strauß, B., Brüggemann, A., Hannover, B., … Rief, W. (2016). Inflation von falsch-positiven Befunden in der psychologischen Forschung: Mögliche Ursachen und Gegenmaßnahmen. Psychologische Rundschau, 67, 163-174.
  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2016). Corrigendum to Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148-174]. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 150.
  • Vuillier, L.*, Bryce, D.*, Whitebread, D., Szucs, D. (2016). The maturation of interference suppression and response inhibition: ERP analysis of a cued Go/Nogo task. PLOS One, 11(11), e0165697. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0165697.*Joint first authors. ()

2015

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Dyjas, O., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Effects of stimulus order on discrimination sensitivity for short and long durations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 1033-1043. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0875-8 ()
  • Birngruber, T., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Introducing a control condition in the classic oddball paradigm: Oddballs are overestimated in duration not only because of their oddness. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 1737-1749. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0868-7 ()
  • Birngruber, T., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2015). The influence of stimulus repetition on duration judgments with simple stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:1213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01213 ()
  • Bryce, D., & Bratzke, D. (2015). Are introspective reaction times affected by the method of time estimation? A comparison of visual analogue scales and reproduction. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 978- 984. doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0804-2 ()
  • Bryce, D., Seifried-Dübon, T., & Bratzke, D. (2015). How are overlapping time intervals perceived? Evidence for a weighted sum of segments model. Acta Psychologica, 156, 83-95. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.12.007 ()
  • Bryce, D., Whitebread, D., & Szucs, D. (2015). The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7 year-old children. Metacognition and Learning, 10(2), 181-198. doi: 10.1007/s11409-014-9120-4 ()
  • Eikmeier, V., Alex-Ruf, S., Maienborn, C., & Ulrich, R. (2015). How strongly linked are mental time and space along the left-right axis? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1878-1883. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000129 ()
  • Eikmeier, V., Hoppe, D., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Response mode does not modulate the space–time congruency effect: Evidence for a space–time mapping at a conceptual level. Acta Psychologica, 156, 162-167. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.10.008 ()
  • Kóbor, A., Takács, Á., Bryce, D., Szűcs, D., Honbolygó, F., Nagy, P., & Csépe, V. (2015). Children with ADHD show impairments in multiple stages of information processing in a Stroop task: An ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 40(6), 329-347. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2015.1086770 ()
  • Maienborn, C., Alex-Ruf, S., Eikmeier, V., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Do we map remembrances to the left/back and expectations to the right/front of a mental timeline? Space–time congruency effects with retrospective and prospective verbs. Acta Psychologica, 156, 168-178. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.006 ()
  • Schröter, H., Birngruber, T., Bratzke, D., Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Task predictability influences the variable foreperiod effect: Evidence of task-specific temporal preparation. Psychological Research, 79, 230-237. doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0550-z ()
  • Schröter, H., Bratzke, D., Fiedler, A., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Does semantic redundancy gain result from multiple semantic priming? Acta Psychologica, 161, 79-85. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.08.001 ()
  • Ulrich, R., Maienborn, C., & Kaup, B. (2015). Understanding the meaning of words and sentences: The role of non-linguistic processes. Acta Psychologica, 156, 97. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.02.016 ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2015). p-Hacking by post hoc selection with multiple opportunities: Detectability by skweness test? --- Comment on Simonsohn, Nelson, and Simmons (2014). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 1137–1145. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148-174. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.02.005 ()

2014

  • Bausenhart, K. M., de la Rosa, M. D., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Multimodal integration of time: Visual and auditory contributions to perceived duration and sensitivity. Experimental Psychology, 61, 310-322. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000249 ()
  • Bausenhart, K. M., Dyjas, O., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Temporal reproductions are influenced by an internal reference: Explaining the Vierordt effect. Acta Psychologica, 147, 60-67. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.011 ()
  • Birngruber, T., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Duration perception of visual and auditory oddball stimuli: Does judgment task modulate the temporal oddball effect? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 814-828. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0602-2 ()
  • Bratzke, D., Bryce, D., & Seifried-Dübon, T. (2014). Distorted subjective reports of stimulus onsets under dual-task conditions: Delayed conscious perception or estimation bias? Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 36-47. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.016 ()
  • Bratzke, D., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2014). The role of consolidation for perceptual learning in temporal discrimination within and across modalities. Acta Psychologica, 147, 75-79. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.018 ()
  • Bryce, D., & Bratzke, D. (2014). Introspective reports of reaction times in dual-tasks reflect experienced difficulty rather than timing of cognitive processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 254 – 267. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.05.011 ()
  • Dietz, P., Ulrich, R., Niess, A., Best, R., Simon, P. & Striegel, H. (2014). Prediction profiles for nutritional supplement use among young German elite athletes. International Journal of Sport Nutrition and Exercise Metabolism, 24, 623-631. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.2014-0009 ()
  • Dyjas, O., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Effects of stimulus order on duration discrimination sensitivity are under attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 292-307. doi: 10.1037/a0033611 ()
  • Dyjas, O., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Effects of stimulus order on discrimination processes in comparative and equality judgements: Data and models. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1121-1150. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.847968 ()
  • Erdfelder, E., Abele-Brehm, A., Blickle, G., Hannover, B., Kirschbaum, C., Möller, J., Rief, W., Strauß, B., Tuschen-Caffner, B., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Bericht des Fachkollegiums Psychologie der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG). DFG-Forschungsförderung in der Psychologie: Aktuelle Herausforderungen und Perspektiven. Psychologische Rundschau, 65, 27-29. doi: 10.1026/0033-3042/a000188 ()
  • Neumann, M., Sterr, A., Claros-Salinas, D., Gütler, R., Ulrich, R., & Dettmers, C. (2014). Modulation of alertness by sustained cognitive demand in MS as surrogate measure of fatigue and fatigability. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 340, 178-182. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.03.024 ()
  • Vatakis, A., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Temporal processing within and across senses. Acta Psychologica, 147, 1. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.001 ()

2013

  • De la Rosa, M. D., & Bausenhart K. M. (2013). Multimodal integration of interval duration: Temporal ventriloquism or changes in pacemaker rate? Timing and Time Perception, 1, 189-215. ()
  • Dietz, P., Striegel, H., Franke, A. G., Lieb, K., Simon, P., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Randomized response estimates for the 12-month prevalence of cognitive-enhancing drug use in university students. Pharmacotherapy, 33, 44-50. ()
  • Dietz, P. Ulrich, R., Dalaker, R. Striegel, H., Franke, A. G., Lieb, K. & Simon. P. (2013). Associations between physical and cognitive doping: A cross-sectional study in 2,997 triathletes. PlosOne, 8(11), e78702. ()
  • Eikmeier, V., Schröter, H., Maienborn, C., Alex-Ruf, S., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Dimensional overlap between time and space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1120-1125. ()
  • Fiedler, A., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Redundancy gain for semantic features. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 474-480. ()
  • Franke, A. G., Bagusat, C., Dietz, P., Hoffmann, I., Simon, P., Ulrich, R., & Lieb, K. (2013). Use of illicit and prescription drugs for cognitive or mood enhancement among surgeons. BMC Medicine, 11: 102. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Mental chronometry and individual differences: Modeling reliabilities and intercorrelations of reaction time means and effect sizes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 819-858. ()

2012

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Dyjas, O., Vorberg, D., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Estimating discrimination performance in two-alternative forced choice tasks: Routines for MATLAB and R. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 1157-1174. ()
  • Bratzke, D., Seifried, T., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Perceptual learning in temporal discrimination: Asymmetric cross-modal transfer from audition to vision. Experimental Brain Research, 221, 205-210. ()
  • Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012a). Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiology International, 29, 55-61. ()
  • Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012b). Reply to: Testing of 'Executive Function'. Chronobiology International, 29, 1285. ()
  • Dyjas, O., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Trial-by-trial updating of an internal reference in discrimination tasks: Evidence from effects of stimulus order and trial sequence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1819-1841. ()
  • Dyjas, O., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wetzels, R., van der Maas, H. L. J., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2012). What’s in a name: A Bayesian hierarchical analysis of the name-letter effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1-14. ()
  • Hoffmann, S., Ulrich, R., & Simon, P. (2012). Refined analysis of the critical age ranges of childhood overweight: Implications for primary prevention. Obesity, 20, 2151-2154. ()
  • Leonhard, T., Bratzke, D., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Time-course analysis of temporal preparation on central processes. Psychological Research, 76, 236-251. ()
  • Rammsayer, T. & Ulrich, R. (2012). The greater temporal acuity in the reminder task than in the 2AFC task is independent of standard duration and sensory modality. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 26-31. ()
  • Steinborn, M. B., Flehmig, H. C., Bratzke, D., & Schröter, H. (2012). Error reactivity in self-paced performance: Highly-accurate individuals exhibit largest post-error slowing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 624-631. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Ruiz Fernández, S., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C. (2012). With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 40, 483-495. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Striegel, H., & Simon, P. (2012). Asking sensitive questions: A statistical power analysis of randomized response models. Psychological Methods, 17, 623-641. ()

2011

  • Bilalic, M., Langner, R., Ulrich, R., & Grodd, W. (2011). Many faces of expertise: Fusiform face area in chess experts and novices. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 10206-10214. ()
  • Brähler, E., Brüggemann, A., Brunstein, J. C., Diehl, M., Erdfelder, E., Kirschbaum, C., Lindenberger, U., Schröger, E., Sonnentag, S., Ulrich, R., & Weber, H. (2011). Bericht des Fachkollegiums Psychologie der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) und der DFG-Geschäftsstelle Psychologie. Psychologische Rundschau, 62, 248-253. ()
  • Fiedler, A., O’Sullivan, J. L., Schröter, H., Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Illusory double flashes can speed up responses like physical ones: evidence from the sound-induced flash illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 214, 113-119. ()
  • Fiedler, A., & Schröter, H. (2011) [invited]. Auditive Fusions- und audiovisuelle Fissionsprozesse [Auditory fusion and audio-visual fission processes]. Lärmbekämpfung – Zeitschrift für Akustik, Schallschutz und Schwingungstechnik, 6, 112-114.
  • Fiedler, A., Schröter, H., Seibold, V. C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). The influence of dichotical fusion on the redundant signals effect, localization performance, and the mismatch negativity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 68-84. ()
  • Fiedler, A., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Coactive processing of dimensionally redundant targets within the auditory modality? Experimental Psychology, 58, 50-54. ()
  • Grondin, S., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Duration discrimination performance: No cross-modal transfer from audition to vision even after massive perceptual learning. In A. Vatakis et al. (Eds.), Multidisciplinary aspects of time and time perception (pp. 92-100). Heidelberg: Springer. ()
  • Leonhard, T., Ruiz Fernández, S., Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2011). Dual-task processing when Task 1 is hard and Task 2 is easy: Reversed central processing order? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 115-136. ()
  • Leonhard, T., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Determinants of central processing order in psychological refractory period paradigms: Central arrival times, detection time, or preparation? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 2012-2043. ()
  • Leuthold, H., & Schröter, H. (2011). Motor programming of finger sequences of different complexity. Biological Psychology, 86, 57-64. ()
  • Rammsayer, T., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Elaborative rehearsal of nontemporal information interferes with temporal processing of durations in the range of seconds but not milliseconds. Acta Psychologica, 137, 127-133. ()
  • Ruiz Fernández, S., Leonhard, T., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Processing two tasks with varying task order: Central stage duration influences central processing order. Acta Psychologica, 137, 10-17. ()
  • Ruiz Fernández, S., Rahona, J. J., Hervás, G., Vázquez, C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Number magnitude determines gaze direction: Spatial-numerical association in a free-choice task. Cortex, 47, 617-620. ()
  • Schröter, H., Fiedler, A., Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Fusion prevents the redundant signals effect: Evidence from stereoscopically presented stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1361-1368. ()
  • Seibold, V. C., Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Does temporal preparation increase the rate of sensory information accumulation? Acta Psychologica, 137, 56-64. ()
  • Seibold, V. C., Fiedler, A., & Rolke, B. (2011). Temporal attention shortens perceptual latency: A temporal prior entry effect. Psychophysiology, 48, 708-717. ()
  • Seifried, T., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Exogenous visual attention prolongs perceived duration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 68-85. ()

2010

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., Seibold, V. C., & Ulrich, R. (2010). Temporal preparation influences the dynamics of information processing: Evidence for early onset of information accumulation. Vision Research, 50, 1024-1035. ()
  • Brähler, E., Brunstein, J. C., Diehl, M., Erdfelder, E., Kirschbaum, C., Lindenberger, U., Schröger, E., Sonnentag, S., Ulrich, R., & Weber, H. (2010). Neuregelung zu Publikationsverzeichnissen bei DFG-Anträgen: Stellungnahme des DFG-Fachkollegiums Psychologie. Psychologische Rundschau, 61, 147-149. ()
  • Claros-Salinas, D., Bratzke, D., Greitemann, G., Nickisch, N., Ochs, L., & Schröter, H. (2010). Fatigue-related diurnal variations of cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis and stroke patients. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 295, 75-81. ()
  • Claros-Salinas, D., Bratzke, D., & Schröter, H. (2010). Subjective and objective assessment of fatigue. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 298, 165-166. ()
  • Langner, R., Steinborn, M. B., Chatterjee, A., Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2010). Mental fatigue and temporal preparation in simple-reaction time performance. Acta Psychologica, 133, 64-72.
  • Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2010). On the locus of temporal preparation: enhancement of pre-motor processes? In A. C. Nobre & J. T. Coull (Eds.), Attention and Time (pp. 227-241). Oxford: University Press. ()
  • Ruiz Fernández, S., & Ulrich, R. (2010). Late backward effects in the refractory period paradigm: Effects of Task 2 execution on Task 1 performance. Psychological Research, 74, 378-387. ()
  • Seifried, T., & Ulrich, R. (2010). Does the asymmetry effect inflate the temporal expansion of odd stimuli? Psychological Research, 74, 90-98. ()
  • Seifried, T., Ulrich, R., Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Osman, A. (2010). Temporal preparation decreases perceptual latency: Evidence from a clock paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2432-2451. ()
  • Steinborn, M. B., Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Gordijn, M. C. M., Beersma, D. G. M., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of 40 hours of constant wakefulness on number comparison performance. Chronobiology International, 27, 807-825. ()
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 134, 94-104. ()
  • Striegel, H., Ulrich, R., & Simon, P. (2010). Randomized response estimates for doping and illicit drug use in elite athletes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 106, 230-232. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (2010). DLs in reminder and 2AFC tasks: Data and models. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1179-1198. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Maienborn, C. (2010). Left-right coding of past and future in language: The mental timeline during sentence processing. Cognition, 117, 126-138. ()

2009

  • Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Steinborn, M. B., & Ulrich, R. (2009). The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task-switching efficiency. Journal of Sleep Research, 18, 167-172. ()
  • Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2009). The source of execution-related dual-task interference: Motor bottleneck or response monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1413-1426. ()
  • Fiedler, A., Schröter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2009). No evidence for a late locus of task switch effects. Brain Research, 1253, 74-80. ()
  • Hackley, S. A., Langner, R., Rolke, B., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Separation of phasic arousal and expectancy effects in a speeded reaction time task via fMRI. Psychophysiology, 46, 163-171. ()
  • Lapid, E., Ulrich, R., & Rammsayer, T. (2009a). Comparisons of two variants of the method of constant stimuli for estimating difference thresholds. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 68, 189-192. ()
  • Lapid, E., Ulrich, R., & Rammsayer, T. (2009b). Perceptual learning in auditory temporal discrimination: No evidence for a cross-modal transfer to the visual modality. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 382-389. ()
  • Miller, J., Beutinger, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Visuospatial attention and redundancy gain. Psychological Research, 73, 254-262. ()
  • Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rolke, B. (2009). On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 273-310. ()
  • Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Schwarz, W. (2009). Why Jackknifing yields good latency estimates. Psychophysiology, 46, 300-312. ()
  • Schröter, H., Frei, L. S., Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2009). The auditory redundant signals effect: An influence of number of stimuli or number of percepts? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1375-1384. ()
  • Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2009). Motor programming of rapid finger sequences: Inferences from movement-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 46, 388-401. ()
  • Steinborn, M. B., Flehmig, H. C., Westhoff, K., & Langner, R. (2009). Differential effects of prolonged work on performance measures in self-paced speed tests. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 5, 105-113.
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132, 40-47. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (2009). Uncovering unobservable cognitive mechanisms: The contribution of mathematical models. In F. Rösler, C. Ranganath, B. Röder, & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Neuroimaging of Human Memory: Linking Cognitive Processes to Neural Systems (pp. 25-41). New York: Oxford University Press. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Vorberg, D. (2009). Estimating the difference limen in 2AFC tasks: Pitfalls and improved estimators. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1219-1227. ()

2008

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Temporal preparation improves temporal resolution: Evidence from constant foreperiods. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1504-1514. ()
  • Bratzke, D., Ulrich, R., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., Jentzsch, I., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1385-1399. ()
  • Lapid, E., Ulrich, R., & Rammsayer, T. (2008). On estimating the difference limen in duration discrimination tasks: A comparison of the 2AFC and the reminder task. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 291-305. ()
  • Miller, J., Sproesser, G., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Constant versus variable response signal delays in speed-accuracy trade-offs: Effects of advance preparation for processing time. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 878-886. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Bimanual response grouping in dual-task paradigms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 999-1019. ()
  • Rolke, B., Dinkelbach, A., Hein, E., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Does attention impair temporal discrimination? Examining non-attentional accounts. Psychological Research, 72, 49-60. ()
  • Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Effects of response sequence length on motor programming: A chronometric analysis. Acta Psychologica, 128, 186-196. ()
  • Steinborn, M. B., Flehmig, H. C., Westhoff, K., & Langner, R. (2008). Predicting school achievement from self-paced continuous performance: Examining the contributions of response speed, accuracy, and response speed variability. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50, 613-634.
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129, 297-307. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (2008). Hören, sehen, denken, handeln – alles gleichzeitig? Aufmerksamkeit und der zentrale Flaschenhals. In: M. Franz, M. Glück-Levi, M. Held & L. Huber (Hrsg.). Hör’ mal schnell --- Zeiten der Aufmerksamkeit (S. 39-53). München: Bayerische Landeszentrale für politische Bildung. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2008). Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 75-121. ()

2007

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Knowing when to hear aids what to hear. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1610-1615. ()
  • Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Ulrich, R., & Peters, M. (2007). Central slowing during the night. Psychological Science, 18, 456-461. ()
  • Flehmig, H. C., Steinborn, M., Langner, R., Scholz, A., & Westhoff, K. (2007). Assessing intraindividual variability in sustained attention: Reliability, relation to speed and accuracy, and practice effects. Psychology Science, 49, 132-149.
  • Flehmig, H. C., Steinborn, M., Langner, R., & Westhoff, K. (2007). Neuroticism and the mental noise hypothesis: Relationship to lapses of attention and slips of action in everyday life. Psychology Science, 49, 343-360.
  • Jentzsch, I., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Decomposing sources of response slowing in the PRP paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 610-626. ()
  • Kiesel, A., Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Systematic biases and Type I error accumulation in tests of the race model inequality. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 539-551. ()
  • Rolke, B., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Impaired temporal discrimination within the attentional blink. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1295-1304. ()
  • Rolke, B., & Hofmann, P. (2007). Temporal uncertainty degrades perceptual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 522-526.
  • Schröter, H., Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2007). Effects of redundant auditory stimuli on reaction time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 39-44. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Miller, J., & Schröter, H. (2007). Testing the race model inequality: An algorithm and computer programs. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 291-302. ()

2006

  • Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., Hackley, S. A., & Ulrich, R. (2006). The locus of temporal preparation effects: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 536-542. ()
  • Bülthoff, H. H., Gillner, S., Mallot, H. A., & Ulrich, R. (2006). Proceedings of the 9th Tübingen Perception Conference. Kirchentellinsfurt: Knirsch.
  • Hein, E., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2006). Visual attention and temporal discrimination: Differential effects of automatic and voluntary cueing. Visual Cognition, 13, 29-50. ()
  • Leuthold, H., & Schröter, H. (2006). Electrophysiological evidence for response priming and conflict regulation in the auditory Simon task. Brain Research, 1097, 167-180. ()
  • Rolke, B., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2006). Attention delays perceived stimulus offset. Vision Research, 46, 2926-2933. ()
  • Schröter, H. (2006). Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1277-1305. ()
  • Simon, P., Striegel, H., Aust, F., Dietz, K., & Ulrich, R. (2006). Doping in fitness sports: Estimated number of unreported cases and individual probability of doping. Addiction, 101, 1640-1644. ()
  • Striegel, H., Simon, P., Frisch, S., Roecker, K., Dietz, K., Dickhuth, H. H., & Ulrich, R. (2006). Anabolic ergogenic substance users in fitness-sports: A distinct group supported by the health care system. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 81, 11-19. ()
  • Striegel, H., Simon, P., Wurster, C., Niess, A. M., & Ulrich, R. (2006). The use of nutritional supplements among master Athlets. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 27, 236-241. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Nitschke, J., & Rammsayer, T. (2006a). Crossmodal temporal discrimination: Assessing the predictions of a general pacemaker-counter model. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1140-1152. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Nitschke, J., & Rammsayer, T. (2006b). Perceived duration of expected and unexpected stimuli. Psychological Research, 70, 77-87. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Ruiz Fernández, S., Jentzsch, I., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2006). Motor limitation in dual-task processing under ballistic movement conditions. Psychological Science, 17, 788-793. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Schröter, H. (2006). Mentale Chronometrie. In J. Funke & P. A. Frensch (Hrsg.), Handbuch der Allgemeinen Psychologie: Kognition (S. 685-690). Göttingen: Hogrefe. ()

2005

  • Bülthoff, H. H., Mallot, H. P., Ulrich, R., & Wichmann, F. A. (2005). Proceedings of the 8th Tübingen Perception Conference. Kirchentellinsfurt: Knirsch.
  • Glaser, W. R., Waschulewski, H., & Schmid, D. (2005). I-TSA – Ein standardisiertes Verfahren zur Bewertung der Auswirkungen von Fahrerinformations- und Fahrerassistenzsystemen auf die Verkehrssicherheit. In VDI Wissensforum (Hrsg.), VDI-Gesellschaft Fahrzeug- und Verkehrstechnik, Fahrer im 21. Jahrhundert. Der Mensch als Fahrer und seine Interaktion mit dem Fahrzeug. Tagung Braunschweig, 22. und 23. November 2005 (S. 17-40). Düsseldorf: VDI Verlag GmbH, VDI Berichte 1919.
  • Rammsayer, T., & Ulrich, R. (2005). No evidence for qualitative differences in the processing of short and long temporal intervals. Acta Psychologica, 120, 141-171. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (2005). Wahrnehmung: Psychologisch und naturwissenschaftlich. In H. D. Betz, D. S. Browing, B. Janowski & E. Jüngel (Hrsg.), Religion in Geschichte und Gegenwart, Band 8 (S. 1259-1261), Tübingen: Mohr Siebeck. ()

2004

  • Bülthoff, H. H., Mallot, H. P., Ulrich, R., & Wichmann, F. A. (2004). Proceedings of the 7th Tübingen Perception Conference. Kirchentellinsfurt: Knirsch.
  • Heil, M., & Rolke, B. (2004). Unattended distractor-induced priming in a visual selective attention task: N400 effects in the absence of RT effects. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 164-169.
  • Heil, M., Rolke, B., & Pecchinenda, A. (2004). Automatic semantic activation is no myth: Semantic context effects on the N400 in the letter-search task in the absence of response time effects. Psychological Science, 15, 852-857.
  • Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (2004). Preparing for action: Inferences from CNV and LRP. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 77-88. ()
  • Miller, J., Kühlwein, E., & Ulrich, R. (2004). Effects of redundant visual stimuli on temporal order judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 563-573. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2004). A computer program for Spearman-Kärber and probit analysis of psychometric function data. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 11-16. ()
  • Niedeggen, M., Heil, M., Ludowig, E., Rolke, B., & Harris, C. L. (2004). Priming illusory words: an ERP approach. Neuropsychologia, 47, 745-753.
  • Rinkenauer, G., Osman, A., Ulrich, R., Müller-Gethmann, H., & Mattes, S. (2004). On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: Inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 261-282. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2004). Threshold estimation in two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) tasks: The Spearman-Kärber method. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 517-533. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Wirtz, M. (2004). On the correlation of a naturally and an artificially dichotomized variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 57, 235-251. ()

2003

  • Bülthoff, H. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Mallot, H. P., Ulrich, R., & Wichmann, F. A. (2003). Beiträge zur 6. Tübinger Wahrnehmungskonferenz. Kirchentellinsfurt: Knirsch.
  • Kaiser, J., Ulrich, R., & Lutzenberger, W. (2003). Dynamics of sensorimotor cortex activation to spatial sounds precueing ipsi- versus contralateral manual responses. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 573-583. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2003). Simple reaction time and statistical facilitation: A parallel grains model. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 101-151. ()
  • Müller-Gethmann, H., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (2003). Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Psychophysiology, 40, 597-611. ()
  • Osman, A., Moore, C. M., & Ulrich, R. (2003). Temporal organization of covert motor processes during response selection and preparation. Biological Psychology, 64, 47-75. ()

2002

  • Bülthoff, H. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Mallot, H. P., & Ulrich, R. (2002). Beiträge zur 5. Tübinger Wahrnehmungskonferenz. Kirchentellinsfurt: Knirsch.
  • Heil, M., & Rolke, B. (2002). Toward a chronopsychophysiology of mental rotation. Psychophysiology, 39, 414-422.
  • Kurz-Milcke, E., & Martignon, L. (2002). Modeling practices and "Tradition". In L. Magnani & N. J. Nersessian (Eds.), Model-Based Reasoning: Science, Technology, Values (pp. 127-146). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
  • Mattes, S., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility in intensity-force relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1175-1191. ()
  • Mattes, S., Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2002). Response force in RT tasks: Isolating effects of stimulus probability and response probability. Visual Cognition, 9, 477-501. ()
  • Miller, J., & Schröter, H. (2002). Online response preparation in a rapid serial visual search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1364-1390. ()
  • Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345-1363. ()

2001

  • Bülthoff, H. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Mallot, H. P., & Ulrich, R. (2001). Beiträge zur 4. Tübinger Wahrnehmungskonferenz. Kirchentellinsfurt: Knirsch.
  • Mattes, S. (2001). Reaktionskraft in Sondierreiz-Aufgaben: Kapazitätseinschränkung, Hemmung oder Flaschenhals? Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 201-206.
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2001). On the analysis of psychometric functions: The Spearman-Kärber method. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1399-1420. ()
  • Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Lamarre, Y. (2001). Locus of the redundant-signals effect in bimodal divided attention: A neurophysiological analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 555-562. ()
  • Rammsayer, T., & Ulrich, R. (2001). Counting models of temporal discrimination. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 270-277. ()
  • Rinkenauer, G., Ulrich, R., & Wing, A. M. (2001). Brief bimanual force pulses: Correlations between the hands in force and time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1485-1497. ()
  • Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., & Hennighausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38, 165-174.
  • Ulrich, R., & Leuthold, H. (2001). Zeitliche und räumliche Handlungsvorbereitung: Chronopsychophysiologische Zugänge. Psychologie und Sport, 4, 149-159. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2001). Using the jackknife-based scoring method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs. Psychophysiology, 38, 816-827. ()
  • Vogt, W., Denzinger, S., Glaser, W. R., Glaser, M. O., & Kuder, T. (2001). Auswirkungen neuer Arbeitskonzepte und insbesondere von Telearbeit auf das Verkehrsverhalten. Bergisch-Gladbach: Berichte der Bundesanstalt für Straßenwesen, Mensch und Sicherheit, Heft M 128.

2000

  • Glaser, W. R. (2000a). Telezentren - Zukunft oder schon Vergangenheit? Telearbeit in Sternenfels. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
  • Glaser, W. R. (2000b). Telezentren - Zukunft oder schon Vergangenheit? In W. R. Glaser (Hrsg.), Telezentren - Zukunft oder schon Vergangenheit? (S. 1-6). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
  • Glaser, W. R., Glaser, M. O., & Kuder, T. (2000). Bevölkerungsbefragung zum Innovationszentrum Fabrik Schweitzer und zu Telearbeit in Sternenfels. In W. R. Glaser (Hrsg.), Telezentren - Zukunft oder schon Vergangenheit? (S. 7-98). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
  • Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Hennighausen, E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-Task: Inhibitory Executive processes? Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 218-225.
  • Müller-Gethmann, H., Rinkenauer, G., Stahl, J., & Ulrich, R. (2000). Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential. Psychophysiology, 37, 507-514. ()
  • Osman, A., Lianggang, L., Müller-Gethmann, H., Rinkenauer, G., Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (2000). Mechanisms of speed-accuracy tradeoff: Evidence from covert motor processes. Biological Psychology, 51, 173-199. ()
  • Rinkenauer, G. (2000). Bimanuelle Koordination isometrischer Kontraktionen: Abhängigkeiten in Kraft und Zeit. Berichte aus der Psychologie. Aachen: Shaker Verlag.
  • Rolke, B., Heil, M., Hennighausen, E., Häussler, C., & Rösler, F. (2000). Topography of brain electrical activity dissociates the sequential order transformation of verbal versus spatial informations in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 282, 81-84.
  • Vogt, W., Denzinger, S., Glaser, W., Glaser, M., & Kuder, T. (2000). Auswirkungen neuer Arbeitskonzepte und insbesondere von Telearbeit auf das Verkehrsverhalten. In Forschungsgesellschaft für Straßen- und Verkehrswesen im Auftrag des Bundesministeriums für Verkehr, Bau und Wohnungswesen (Hrsg.), Informationen, Forschung im Straßen- und Verkehrswesen, Teil Stadtverkehr (S. 545-547).

1999

  • Dufft, C., & Ulrich, R. (1999). Intersensorische Erleichterung: Auch visuelle Begleitreize können die Reaktionszeit verkürzen. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 46, 16-27. ()
  • Glaser, W. R. (1999). Telearbeit - große Prognosen und kleine Veränderungen. In N. Streitz, B. Remmers, M. Pietzcker & R. Grundmann (Hrsg.), Arbeitswelten im Wandel - fit für die Zukunft (S. 104-126). Stuttgart: Deutsche Verlags-Anstalt.
  • Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1999). Wie ändert sich das Kommunikationsmuster bei alternierender Telearbeit? Eine Kommunikationsanalyse. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationsychologie, 43, 134-141.
  • Glaser, W. R., & Vogt, W. (1999). Ergebnisse aus dem Forschungs- und Entwicklungsvorhaben "Auswirkungen neuer Arbeitskonzepte und insbesondere von Telearbeit auf das Verkehrsverhalten". In Bundesministerium für Wirtschaft und Technologie (Hrsg.), Telearbeit im Mittelstand, Erfahrungen aus der Praxis (S. 217-224). Berlin: Herausgeber. BMWI Dokumentation Nr. 467.
  • Heil, M., Rolke, B., Engelkamp, J., Rösler, F., Özcan, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). Event-related brain potentials during recognition of ordinary and bizarre action phrases following verbal and subject-performed encoding conditions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 261-280.
  • Miller, J., Franz, V., & Ulrich, R. (1999). Effects of auditory stimulus intensity on response force in simple, go/no-go, and choice tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 107-119. ()
  • Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (1999). Effects of stimulus intensity on the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1454-1471. ()
  • Rimpler, I., Glaser, M. O., & Glaser, W. R. (1999). Praxisbeispiel Telearbeit: Von der Arbeitsstätte zum Kommunikationszentrum. Office Management, 2, 42-45.
  • Rimpler, I., Glaser, M. O., & Glaser, W. R. (1999). Das Büro für mobiles Arbeiten: Von der Arbeitsstätte zum Kommunikationszentrum. Erfahrungen mit dem Business-Center von DIGITAL Equipment GmbH. In B. Godehardt, W. B. Korte, U. Michelsen & H.-P. Quadt (Hrsg.), Managementhandbuch Telearbeit (250.2, 1-15). Heidelberg: Hüthig Verlag.
  • Rinkenauer, G., Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1999). The surface-weight illusion: On the contribution of grip force to preceived heaviness. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 23-30. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Mattes, S., & Miller, J .O. (1999). Donder's assumption of pure insertion: An evaluation on the basis of response dynamics. Acta Psychologica, 102, 43-75. ()

1998

  • Glaser, W. R. (1998). Telearbeit - Ein Weg in die Informationsgesellschaft. In B. Dorn (Hrsg.), Cyberbeben. Was die multimediale Revolution für Unternehmen und Märkte bedeutet (S. 267-281). Wiesbaden: Betriebswirtschaftlicher Verlag Dr. Th. Gabler GmbH.
  • Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Directed attention prolongs the perceived duration of a brief stimulus. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1305-1317. ()
  • Miller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology, 35, 99-115. ()
  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Locus of the effect of the number of alternative responses: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1215-1231. ()
  • Stief, V., Leuthold, H., Miller, J., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (1998). The effect of response complexity on the lateralized readiness potential. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 206, 305-319. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Leuthold, H., & Sommer, W. (1998). Motor programming of response force and movement direction. Psychophysiology, 35, 721-728. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Rinkenauer, G., & Miller, J. (1998). Effects of stimulus duration and intensity on simple reaction time and response force. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 915-928. ()
  • Wing, A. M., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (1998). Variability associated with cyclic changing of target force in rhythmic serial responding. Current Psychology of Cognition, 17, 781-790. ()

1997

  • Glaser, W. R. (1997a). Telearbeit bei IBM - Erfahrungen aus einem Pilotprojekt in Baden-Württemberg. In: Institut für Landes- und Stadtentwicklungsforschung des Landes Nordrhein-Westfalen (Hrsg.), Daten- statt Autobahn? Entwicklungsperspektiven und Handlungserfordernisse im Zusammenhang von Telematik, räumlicher Struktur und Verkehr (S. 71-80). Dortmund: Herausgeber.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1997b). Auswirkungen der Telekooperation auf die Arbeitswelt. In B. Godehardt, W. B. Korte, U. Michelsen, & H.-P. Quadt (Hrsg.), Managementhandbuch Telearbeit (280.3, 1-22). Heidelberg: Hüthig Verlag.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1997c). Systemtheorie. In J. Straub, W. Kempf & H. Werbik (Hrsg.), Psychologie. Eine Einführung (S. 68-96). München: Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1997d). Wahrnehmung. In J. Straub, W. Kempf & H. Werbik (Hrsg.), Psychologie. Eine Einführung (S. 225-248). München: Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag.
  • Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1997). Response force is sensitive to the temporal uncertainty of response stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1089-1097. ()
  • Mattes, S., Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (1997). Effects of response probability on response force in simple RT. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 405-420. ()
  • Mattes, S., Ulrich, R., & Mühlbradt, L. (1997). Detection times of natural teeth and Endosseous Implants revealed by the method of reaction time. The International Journal of Oral & Maxillofacial Implants, 12, 399-402. (This work was also translated into Japanese by the Society of Dental Implants and this translation appeared in Quintessence Dental Implantology). ()
  • Rinkenauer, G., Ulrich, R., & Wing, A. M. (1996). Zentrale und periphere Quellen der Variabilität von isometrischen Bewegungen. In E. Loosch & M. Tamme (Hrsg.), Motorik - Struktur und Funktion (S. 83-86). Hamburg: Czwalina. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (1997). Tests of race models for reaction time in experiments with asynchronous redundant signals. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 41, 367-381. ()

1996

  • Glaser, W. R. (1996). Repräsentation bei Maschinen. Zeitschrift für Semiotik, 18, 169-189.
  • Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (1996). Partial advance information and response preparation: Inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 307-323. ()
  • Sommer, U., Ulrich, R., & Leuthold, H. (1996). Das Lateralisierte Bereitschaftspotential als psychophysiologischer Zugang bei der Untersuchung kognitiver Prozesse. Psychologische Rundschau, 47, 1-14. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Giray, M., & Stapf, K.-H. (1996). Einprägen, Vergessen und Erinnern. In D. Albert & K.-H. Stapf (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Gedächtnis (Themenbereich C: Theorie und Forschung, Serie II: Kognition, Band 4) (S. 95-179). Göttingen: Hogrefe. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Mattes, S. (1996). Does immediate arousal enhance response force in simple reaction time? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 972-990. ()

1995

  • Glaser, W. R. (1995a). Telearbeit. Praxis statt Utopie auf dem Weg in die Informationsgesell­schaft. Bertelsmann Briefe, 134, 19-23.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1995b). Anmerkungen zur Telearbeit: "Solche wirklich grundlegenden Änderungen haben noch immer die Generationen als Zeitmaß". Telekom Praxis, 9, 18-24.
  • Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1995). Telearbeit in der Praxis. Psychologische Erfahrungen mit Außerbetrieblichen Arbeitsstätten bei der IBM Deutschland GmbH. Neuwied: Luchterhand.
  • Osman, A., Moore, C. M., & Ulrich, R. (1995). Bisecting RT with lateralized readiness potentials: Precue effects after LRP onset. Acta Psychologica, 90, 111-112. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Wing, A. M., & Rinkenauer, G. (1995). Duration and amplitude scaling of the mean force-time function of brief isometric force pulses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1457-1472. ()

1994

  • Glaser, W. R. (1994a). Arnold Gehlens Anthropologie und die Psychologie der menschlichen In­formationsverarbeitung. In: H. Klages, & H. Quaritsch (Hrsg.), Zur geisteswissenschaftli­chen Bedeutung Arnold Gehlens. Vorträge und Diskussionsbeiträge des Sonderseminars 1989 der Hochschule für Verwaltungswissenschaften Speyer (S. 377-396). Berlin: Duncker und Humblot.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1994b). Menschliche Informationsverarbeitung. In: E. Eberleh, H. Oberquelle, & R. Oppermann (Hrsg.), Einführung in die Software-Ergonomie (S. 7-51). Berlin: De Gruyter.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1994c). Teleworking - Erfahrungen mit dezentralen Arbeitsplätzen bei der IBM. In: Telak GmbH (Hrsg.), Corporate Networks und neue Techniken. Proceedings des Telekom-Anwender-Kongress '94 (S. 27-40). Braunschweig: Vieweg.
  • Mühlbradt, L., Mattes, S., Möhlmann, H., & Ulrich, R. (1994). Touch sensitivity of natural teeth and endosseous implants revealed by difference thresholds. The International Journal of Oral & Maxillofacial Implants, 9, 412-416. (This work was also translated into Japanese by the Society of Dental Implants and this translation appeared in Quintessence Dental Implantology) ()
  • Sommer, W., Leuthold, H., & Ulrich, R. (1994). The lateralized readiness potential preceding brief isometric force pulses of different peak force and rate of force production. Psychophysiology, 31, 503-512. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (1994). Effects of truncation on reaction time analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 34-80. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Stapf, K.-H., & Zachay, A. (1994). Kategoriale Gedächtnislisten und Abrufprozesse im Kurzzeitgedachtnis bei älteren und jüngeren Menschen. Sprache und Kognition, 13, 146-159. ()

1993

  • Giray, M., & Ulrich, R. (1993). Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 1278-1291. ()
  • Glaser, W. R. (1993a). Außerbetriebliche Arbeitsstätten - psychologisch, praktisch und ein wenig visionär gesehen. IBM Nachrichten 43, 315, 15-21.
  • Glaser, W. R. (1993b). Why does interference in an interlingual Stroop-like word-word task sometimes disappear? Comment on S. Ikeda's article. Hiroshima Forum for Psychology, 15, 29-31.
  • Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1993). Colors as properties: Stroop-like effects between objects and their colors. In G. Strube, & K. F.Wender (Eds.), The cognitive psychology of knowledge. Advances in Psychology Series 101 (pp. 1-32). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
  • Mühlbradt, L., Mattes, S., Ulrich, R., & Möhlmann, H. (1993). Zur zeitlichen Stabilität von Tastschwellen bei Tübinger Implantaten und natürlichen Zähnen. Zeitschrift für zahnärztliche Implantologie, 9, 139-143. ()
  • Mühlbradt, L., Ulrich, R., Möhlmann, H., Mattes, S., & Sippel, T. (1993). Einfluß der Anästhesie auf den Tastsinn von Tübinger Implantaten und natürlichen Zähnen. Zeitschrift für zahnärztliche Implantologie, 9, 66-70. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (1993). Information processing models generating lognormally distributed reaction times. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 513-525. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Moore, C., & Osman, A. (1993). Lateralisiertes Bereitschaftspotential und Reaktionsgeschwindigkeit bei partieller Vorinformation eines Handlungsparameters. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 40, 310-325. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Mühlbradt, L., Möhlmann, H., & Schmid, H. (1993). Qualitative aspects of mechanoperception with natural teeth versus enosseous implants revealed by factor analysis. The International Journal of Oral & Maxillofacial Implants, 8, 173-178.
  • Ulrich, R., & Wing, A. (1993). Variability of brief force impulses. In: K. M. Newell & D. M. Corcos (Eds.), Variability and motor control (pp. 37-51). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. ()

1992

  • Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61-105. Reprinted in Levelt, W. J. M. (Ed.), Lexical access in speech production (pp. 61-105). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

1991

  • Elbert, T. A. , Ulrich, R., Rockstroh, B., & Lutzenberger, W. (1991). The processing of temporal intervals reflected by CNV-like brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 28, 648-655. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Wing, A. M. (1991). A recruitment theory of force-time relations in the production of brief force pulses: The parallel force unit model. Psychological Review, 98, 268-294. ()

1990

  • Mühlbradt, L., Ulrich, R., Möhlmann, H., Schmid, H., & Wendler, K. (1990). Die Wahrnehmung von überschwelligen Kräften an enossalen Implantaten und natürlichen Zähnen. Zeitschrift für zahnärztliche Implantologie, 6, 161-165. ()
  • Ulrich, R., Giray, M., & Schäffer, R. (1990). Is it possible to prepare the second component of a movement before the first one? Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 125-148. ()

1989

  • Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1989). Context effects in Stroop-like word and picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 13-42.
  • Mühlbradt, L., Ulrich, R., Möhlmann, H., & Schmid, H. (1989a). Adaptive Bestimmung der Tastschwellen bei natürlichen Zähnen und enossalen Implantaten. Zeitschrift für zahnärztliche Implantologie, 5, 101-107. ()
  • Mühlbradt, L., Ulrich, R., Möhlmann, H., & Schmid, R. (1989b). Computergestützte Verfahren zur Tastschwellenbestimmung bei natürlichen Zähnen und enossalen Implantaten. Orale Implantologie, 1, 27-30. ()
  • Mühlbradt, L., Ulrich, R., Möhlmann, H., & Schmid, H. (1989c). Mechanoperception of natural teeth versus endosseous implants revealed by magnitude estimation. The International Journal of Oral & Maxillofacial Implants, 4, 125-130. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Giray, M. (1989). Time resolution of clocks: Effects on reaction time measurement - Good news for bad clocks. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 42, 1-12. ()

1988

  • Glaser, W. R. (1988). Technical improvements to the projection tachistoscope. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20, 491-494.

1987

  • Ulrich, R. (1987). Threshold models of temporal-order judgments evaluated by a ternary response task. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 224-239. ()
  • Vorberg, D., & Ulrich, R. (1987). Random search with unequal search rates: Serial and parallel generalizations of McGill's model. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 31, 1-23. ()

1986

  • Muthig, K.-P., & Ulrich, R. (1986). Vorab-Einrichtungen von Suchbereichen im KZG. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 194, 63-72. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Giray, M. (1986). Separate-activation models with variable base times: Testability and checking of cross-channel dependency. Perception & Psychophysics, 39, 248-254. ()

1985

  • Muthig, K.-P., & Ulrich, R. (1985). Selective search in short-term memory under ideal conditions of test stimulus categorization. Memory & Cognition, 13, 29-36. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (1985a). Die Beziehung zwischen Testlänge und Validität für nicht-parallele Aufgaben: Verschiedene Methoden der Validitätsmaximierung. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 6, 32-45. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (1985b). Der zeitliche Verlauf der freien Reproduktion bei kategorialen Wortlisten: Der Einfluss der semantischen Ähnlichkeit auf den Reproduktionsverlauf. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 32, 642-664. ()
  • Ulrich, R. (1985c). Erwiderung auf Mosers Diskussionsbeitrag: "Lehrstunde in Testtheorie?". Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 4, 237-240. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Dietz, K. (1985). The short-term storage as a buffer memory between long-term storage and the motor system: A simultaneous-processing model. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 29, 243-270. ()

1984

  • Glaser, W. R., & Düngelhoff, F.-J. (1984). The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 640-654.
  • Ulrich, R., & Giray, M. (1984). Improved algorithms and a Fortran program (MAXVAL) for selecting the most valid items from a pretested item pool (Bericht Nr. 13). Tübingen: Psychologisches Institut der Universität Tübingen. ()
  • Ulrich, R., & Stapf, K. H. (1984). A double-response paradigm to study stimulus intensity effects upon the motor system in simple reaction time experiments. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 545-558. ()

1983

  • Ulrich, R., & Muthig, K.-P. (1983). Darbietungsabhängige Vergleichsprozesse bei kategorial zerlegbaren Gedächtnismengen. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 30, 673-694. ()