Publications

Seibold, V.C., Balke, J., & Rolke, B. (2023). Temporal attention. Frontiers in Cognition, doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2023.1168320

Rolke, B., Kirsten, M., Seibold, V. C., Dietrich, S., & Hertrich, I.  (2023). Processing references in context: when the polar bear does not meet a polar bear. Cognitive Processing, doi: 10.1007/s10339-023-01150-4

Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Blum, C., Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B.  (2023). The cortical dynamics of context-dependent language processing, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, doi: 10.1080/23273798.2023.216512

Balke, J., Rolke, B., & Seibold, V. C. (2022). Temporal preparation accelerates spatial selection by facilitating bottom-up processing. Brain research, 1777. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2021.147765.

Hochmitz, I, Hein, E., & Yeshurun, Y. (21). The effects of spatial attention on temporal integration measured with the Ternus display. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

Hein, E., Stepper, M. Y., Hollingworth, A., & Moore, C. M. (2021). Visual working memory content influences correspondence processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,47 (3), 331-343.

Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., Blum, C. & Ackermann, H. (2021). The Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex for Speech and Language Processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:645209. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.645209 

Balke, J., Rolke, B., & Seibold, V. C. (2021). Reduction of temporal uncertainty facilitates stimulus-driven processes in spatial selection. Biological Psychology, 159. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108028.

Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., & Ackermann, H. (2020). The margins of the language network in the brain. Front. Commun., 5:519955. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2020519955

Stepper, M.Y., Moore, C. M., Rolke, B., & Hein, E. (2020). Illusory size determines the perception of ambiguous apparent motion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01786-9

Seibold, V. C., Stepper, M. Y., & Rolke, B. (2020). Temporal attention boosts perceptual effects of spatial attention and feature-based attention. Brain and Cognition,142.  doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105570

Moore, C. M., Stephens, T., & Hein, E. (2020). Object correspondence: Using perceived causality to infer how the visual system knows what went where. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 181-192. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01763-y

Stepper, M.Y., Moore, C. M., Rolke, B., & Hein, E. (2019). The role of object history in establishing object correspondence. , Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 82, 1038-1050. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01923-0

Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Seibold, V.C., & Rolke, B. (2019). Discourse management during speech perception: a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. NeuroImage. 202, 116047. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116047

Seibold, V.C., Dietrich, S., & Rolke, B. (2019). Multi-dimensional selection by temporal attention and feature-based attention: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146340

Stepper, M.Y., Rolke, B., & Hein, E. (2019). How voluntary spatial attention influences feature biases in object correspondence. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 82, 1024-1037. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01801-9

 

Rolke, B., Stepper, M. Y., Seibold, V. C., & Hein, E. (2019). Aesthetic stimuli attract visual spatial attention. Art & Perception, 7, 52-81. doi: 10.1163/22134913-20191101

Hein, E. & Schütz, A. C. (2019). Competition between color and luminance in motion correspondence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 310-322. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-018-1589-5

Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., Ackerman, H. (2018). Cortical phase locking to accelerated speech in blind and sighed listeners prior to and after training. Brain and Language, 185, 19-29. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.07.002

Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Müller-Dahlhaus, F., Ackermann, H., Belardinelli, P., Desideri, D., Seibold, V. C., & Ziemann, U. (2018). Reduced performance during a sentence repetition task by continuous theta-burst magnetic stimulation of the pre-supplementary motor area. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 361. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00361

Seibold, V. C. (2018). Do alerting signals increase the size of the attentional focus? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 402-425. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1451-1

Hein, E. (2017). Chapter 99: The Ternus effect. In A. G. Shapiro & D. Todorovic (Eds.), The Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions (pp. 685-691). New York: Oxford University Press.

Lohmann, J., Rolke, B., & Butz, M. (2017). In touch with mental rotation: Interactions between mental and tactile rotations and motor responses. Experimental Brain Research, 235, 1063–1079. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4861-8

Hein, E., Blaschke, S., & Rolke, B. (2017). The influence of object similarity and orientation on object-based cueing. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79, 63-77. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1229-x. http://rdcu.be/l8Ll

Rolke, B., Festl, F., & Seibold, V. C. (2016). Toward the influence of temporal attention on the selection of targets in a visual search task: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 53, 1690-1701. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12734

Belardinelli, A., Stepper, M. Y., & Butz, M. V. (2016). It's in the eyes: Planning precise manual actions before execution. Journal of vision, 16(1), 1-18. doi:10.1167/16.1.18

Hertrich, I., Kirsten, M., Tiemann, S., Beck, S., Wuehle, A., Ackermann, H., & Rolke, B. (2015). Context-dependent impact of presuppositions on early magnetic brain responses during speech perception. Brain and Language, 149, 1-12. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.06.005

Tiemann, S., Kirsten, M., Beck, S., Hertrich, I., & Rolke, B. (2015). Presupposition processing and accommodation: An experiment on wieder (‘again’) and consequences for other triggers. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental Perspectives on Presuppositions: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics (pp. 39-65). Heidelberg: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-07980-6_2

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2014). Evidence for scene-based motion correspondence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 793-804. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0616-9

Rolke, B., Ruiz Fernández, S., Seibold, V. C., & Rahona, J. J. (2014). Crossed hands stay on the time-line. Cognitive Processing, 15(Suppl 1), 134-135.doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0632-2

Seibold V. C., Festl, F., & Rolke, B. (2014). Temporal preparation increases response conflict by faster direct response activation. Cognitive Processing, 15(Suppl 1), 67-68. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0632-2

Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B. (2014). Does temporal preparation facilitate visual processing in a selective manner? Evidence from attentional capture. Acta Psychologica, 137, 51-61. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.012

Kirsten, M., Tiemann, S., Seibold, V. C., Hertrich, I. Beck, S., & Rolke, B. (2014). When the polar bear encounters many polar bears: ERP context effects evoked by uniqueness failure. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 29, 1147-1162. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.899378

Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B. (2014). Does temporal preparation speed up visual processing? Evidence from the N2pc. Psychophysiology, 51, 529-538. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12196

Rahona, J. J., Ruiz Fernández, S., Rolke, B., Vázquez, C., & Hervás, G. (2014). Overt head movements moderate the effect of depressive symptoms on mood regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1328-1337. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.881323

Rolke, B., Ruiz Fernández, S., Schmid, M., Walker, M., Lachmair, M., Rahona López, J. J., Hervás, G., & Vázquez, C. (2013). Priming the mental time-line: Effects of modality and processing mode. Cognitive Processing, 14, 231-244. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0537-5

Ruiz Fernández, S., Leonhard, T., Lachmaier, M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Processing order in dual-tasks when the duration of motor responses varies. Universitas Psychologica, 12(5), 442-449. doi:10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-5.podt

Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Reply to: Testing of `Executive Function´. Chronobiology International, 29, 1285. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.728412

Rolke, B. (2012). Der Ausbruch des Gedanken - der Mensch als Randwesen der Evolution (The outpouring of thoughts: Humanity on the margins of evolution). In M. Flacke (Ed.), Verführung Freiheit: Kunst in Europa seit 1945, XXX. Europaratsausstellung (pp. 284-307). Berlin: Deutsches Historisches Museum.

Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and the Simon task. Chronobiology International, 29, 55-61. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.635235

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2012). Spatio-temporal priority revisited: The role of feature identity and similarity for object correspondence in apparent motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 975-988. doi: 10.1037/a0028197

Hein, E., & Cavanagh, P. (2012). Motion correspondence shows feature bias in spatiotopic coordinates. Journal of Vision, 12(7), 16. doi: 10.1167/12.7.16, http://journalofvision.org/12/7/16/

Festl, F., Recktenwald, F., Yuan, C., & Mallot, H. A. (2012). Detection of linear ego-acceleration from optic flow. Journal of Vision, 12(7), 10. doi: 10.1167/12.7.10, http://journalofvision.org/12/7/10

Tiemann, S., Schmid, M., Bade, N., Rolke, B., Hertrich, I., Ackermann, H., Knapp, J., & Beck, S. (2011). Psycholinguistic evidence for presuppositions: On-line and off-line data. In I. Reich, E. Horch, & D. Pauly (Eds.), Proceedings of Sinn & Bedeutung 15 (pp. 581-595). Saarbrücken: Universaar – Saarland Unversity Press.

Ruiz-Fernandez, S., Leonhard, T., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Processing two task with varying task order: Central stage duration influences central processing order. Acta Psychologica, 137, 10-17. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.016

Seibold, V. C., Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Does temporal preparation increase the rate of sensory information accumulation? Acta Psychologica, 137, 56-64. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.02.006.

Heil, M., Kavsek, M., Rolke, B., Beste, C., & Jansen, P. (2011). Mental rotation in female fraternal twins: Evidence for intra-uterine hormone transfer? Biological Psychology, 86, 90-93. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.11.002

Seibold, V. C., Fiedler, A., & Rolke, B. (2011). Temporal attention shortens perceptual latency: A temporal prior entry effect. Psychophysiology, 48, 708-717. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01135.x

Steinborn, M. B., & Langner, R. (2011). Distraction by irrelevant sound during foreperiods selectively impairs temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 136, 405-418. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.008

Yeshurun, Y., & Hein, E. (2011). Transient attention degrades perceived apparent motion. Perception, 40, 905-918. doi: 10.1068/p7016

Fiedler, A., Schröter, H., Seibold, V. C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). The auditory redundant signals effect depends on the number of percepts: Evidence from the mismatch negativity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 68-84. doi: 10.3758/s13415-010-0013-y

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., Seibold, V. C., & Ulrich, R. (2010). Temporal preparation influences the dynamics of information processing: Evidence for early onset of information accumulation. Vision Research, 50, 1025-1034. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.011

Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2010). On the locus of temporal preparation: Enhancement of pre-motor processes. In A. C. Nobre & J. T. Coull (Eds.), Attention and time (pp. 227-241). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Seifried, T., Ulrich, R., Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Osman, A. (2010). Temporal preparation decreases perceptual latency: Evidence from a clock paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2431-2451. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.485354

Steinborn, M. B., Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Gordijn, M. C. M., Beersma, D. G. M., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of 40 hours of constant wakefulness on number comparison performance. Chronobiology International, 27, 807-825. doi: 10.3109/07420521003778765

Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 134, 94-104. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.011

Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Steinborn, M. B., & Ulrich, R. (2009). The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task-switching efficiency. Journal of Sleep Research, 18, 167-172. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00729.x

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2010). Investigating temporal properties of covert shifts of visual attention using the attentional walk task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 41-46. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.1.41

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2010). Unmasking the standing wave of invisibility: An account in terms of apparent motion and representational updating. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 398-408. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.2.398

Moore, C. M., Stephens, T. M., & Hein, E. (2010). Features, as well as space and time guide object persistence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 731-736. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.5.731

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2010). Lateral masking in cycling displays: the relative importance of separation, flanker duration and inter-stimulus interval. Perception, 39, 1330-1340. doi: 10.1068/p6643

Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2009). The source of execution-related dual-task interference: Motor bottleneck or response monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1413-1426. doi: 10.1037/a0015874

Hackley, S. A., Langner, R., Rolke, B., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Separation of phasic arousal and expectancy effects in a speeded reaction time task via fMRI. Psychophysiology, 46, 163-171. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00722.x

Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rolke, B. (2009). On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 273-310. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.08.003

Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132, 40-47. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.002

Moore, C. M., Hein, E., Grosjean, M., & Rinkenauer, G. (2009). Limited influence of perceptual organization on the precision of attentional control. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 971-983. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.4.971

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2009). Explicit eye movements failed to facilitate the precision of later attentional localization. Experimental Brain Research, 194, 387-394. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1927-x

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Temporal preparation improves temporal resolution: Evidence from constant foreperiods. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1504-1514. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.8.1504

Bratzke, D., Ulrich, R., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., Jentzsch, I., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1385-1399. doi: 10.1080/17470210701536856

Rolke, B. (2008). Temporal preparation facilitates perceptual identification of letters. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1305-1313. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.7.1305

Rolke, B., Dinkelbach, A., Hein, E., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Does attention impair temporal discrimination? Examining non-attentional accounts. Psychological Research, 72, 49-60. doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0092-0

Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129, 297-307. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.08.005

Rolke, B., & Hofmann, P. (2007). Temporal uncertainty degrades perceptual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 522-526. doi: 10.3758/BF03194101

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Knowing when to hear aids what to hear. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1610-1615. doi: 0.1080/17470210701536419

Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Ulrich, R., & Peters, M. (2007). Central slowing during the night. Psychological Science, 18, 456-461. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01921.x

Rolke, B., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Impaired temporal discrimination within the attentional blink. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1295-1304. doi: 10.3758/BF03192946

Hein, E., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2006). Visual attention and temporal discrimination: Differential effects of automatic and voluntary cueing. Visual Cognition, 13, 29-50. doi: 10.1080/13506280500143524

Rolke, B., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2006). Attention delays perceived stimulus offset. Vision Research, 46, 2926-2933. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.02.022

Ulrich, R., Fernandez, S. R., Jentzsch, I., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2006). Motor limitation in dual-task processing under ballistic movement conditions. Psychological Science, 17, 788-793. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01783.x

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., Hackley, S. A., & Ulrich, R. (2006). The locus of temporal preparation effects: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 536-542. doi: 10.3758/BF03193882

Rolke, B. (2003). Ein Wort und seine Folgen. Gehirn & Geist, 3, 73-75.

Heil, M., & Rolke, B. (2004). Unattended distractor-induced priming in a visual selective attention task - N400 effects in the absence of RT effects. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 164-169. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.18.4.164

Heil, M., Rolke, B., & Pecchinenda, A. (2004). Automatic semantic activation is no myth - Semantic context effects on the N400 in the letter-search task in the absence of response time effects. Psychological Science, 15, 852-857. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00766.x

Niedeggen, M., Heil, M., Ludowig, E., Rolke, B., & Harris, C. L. (2004). Priming illusory words: an ERP approach. Neuropsychologia, 42, 745-753. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.007

Heil, M., Rosler, F., & Rolke, B. (2003). Another artificial division - and the data don't support it. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 739-740. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0333016X

Heil, M., & Rolke, B. (2002). Toward a chronopsychophysiology of mental rotation. Psychophysiology, 39, 414-422. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3940414

Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., & Hennighausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38, 165-174. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3820165

Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Hennighausen, E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: Inhibitory executive processes? Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 218-225. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.14.4.218

Rolke, B., Heil, M., Hennighausen, E., Haussler, C., & Rösler, F. (2000). Topography of brain electrical activity dissociates the sequential order transformation of verbal versus spatial information in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 282, 81-84. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00891-0

Heil, M., Rolke, B., Engelkamp, J., Rosler, F., Ozcan, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). Event-related brain potentials during recognition of ordinary and bizarre action phrases following verbal and subject-performed encoding conditions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 261-280. doi: 10.1080/713752313