Publications

Seibold, V.C., Balke, J., & Rolke, B. (2023). Temporal attention. Frontiers in Cognition, doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2023.1168320

Rolke, B., Kirsten, M., Seibold, V. C., Dietrich, S., & Hertrich, I.  (2023). Processing references in context: when the polar bear does not meet a polar bear. Cognitive Processing, doi: 10.1007/s10339-023-01150-4

Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Blum, C., Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B.  (2023). The cortical dynamics of context-dependent language processing, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, doi: 10.1080/23273798.2023.216512

Balke, J., Rolke, B., & Seibold, V. C. (2022). Temporal preparation accelerates spatial selection by facilitating bottom-up processing. Brain research, 1777. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2021.147765.

Hochmitz, I, Hein, E., & Yeshurun, Y. (21). The effects of spatial attention on temporal integration measured with the Ternus display. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

Hein, E., Stepper, M. Y., Hollingworth, A., & Moore, C. M. (2021). Visual working memory content influences correspondence processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,47 (3), 331-343.

Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., Blum, C. & Ackermann, H. (2021). The Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex for Speech and Language Processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:645209. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.645209 

Balke, J., Rolke, B., & Seibold, V. C. (2021). Reduction of temporal uncertainty facilitates stimulus-driven processes in spatial selection. Biological Psychology, 159. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108028.

Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., & Ackermann, H. (2020). The margins of the language network in the brain. Front. Commun., 5:519955. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2020519955

Stepper, M.Y., Moore, C. M., Rolke, B., & Hein, E. (2020). Illusory size determines the perception of ambiguous apparent motion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01786-9

Seibold, V. C., Stepper, M. Y., & Rolke, B. (2020). Temporal attention boosts perceptual effects of spatial attention and feature-based attention. Brain and Cognition,142.  doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105570

Moore, C. M., Stephens, T., & Hein, E. (2020). Object correspondence: Using perceived causality to infer how the visual system knows what went where. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 181-192. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01763-y

Stepper, M.Y., Moore, C. M., Rolke, B., & Hein, E. (2019). The role of object history in establishing object correspondence. , Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 82, 1038-1050. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01923-0

Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Seibold, V.C., & Rolke, B. (2019). Discourse management during speech perception: a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. NeuroImage. 202, 116047. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116047

Seibold, V.C., Dietrich, S., & Rolke, B. (2019). Multi-dimensional selection by temporal attention and feature-based attention: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146340

Stepper, M.Y., Rolke, B., & Hein, E. (2019). How voluntary spatial attention influences feature biases in object correspondence. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 82, 1024-1037. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01801-9

 

Rolke, B., Stepper, M. Y., Seibold, V. C., & Hein, E. (2019). Aesthetic stimuli attract visual spatial attention. Art & Perception, 7, 52-81. doi: 10.1163/22134913-20191101

Hein, E. & Schütz, A. C. (2019). Competition between color and luminance in motion correspondence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 310-322. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-018-1589-5

Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., Ackerman, H. (2018). Cortical phase locking to accelerated speech in blind and sighed listeners prior to and after training. Brain and Language, 185, 19-29. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.07.002

Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Müller-Dahlhaus, F., Ackermann, H., Belardinelli, P., Desideri, D., Seibold, V. C., & Ziemann, U. (2018). Reduced performance during a sentence repetition task by continuous theta-burst magnetic stimulation of the pre-supplementary motor area. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 361. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00361

Seibold, V. C. (2018). Do alerting signals increase the size of the attentional focus? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 402-425. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1451-1

Hein, E. (2017). Chapter 99: The Ternus effect. In A. G. Shapiro & D. Todorovic (Eds.), The Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions (pp. 685-691). New York: Oxford University Press.

Lohmann, J., Rolke, B., & Butz, M. (2017). In touch with mental rotation: Interactions between mental and tactile rotations and motor responses. Experimental Brain Research, 235, 1063–1079. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4861-8

Hein, E., Blaschke, S., & Rolke, B. (2017). The influence of object similarity and orientation on object-based cueing. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79, 63-77. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1229-x. http://rdcu.be/l8Ll

Rolke, B., Festl, F., & Seibold, V. C. (2016). Toward the influence of temporal attention on the selection of targets in a visual search task: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 53, 1690-1701. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12734

Belardinelli, A., Stepper, M. Y., & Butz, M. V. (2016). It's in the eyes: Planning precise manual actions before execution. Journal of vision, 16(1), 1-18. doi:10.1167/16.1.18

Hertrich, I., Kirsten, M., Tiemann, S., Beck, S., Wuehle, A., Ackermann, H., & Rolke, B. (2015). Context-dependent impact of presuppositions on early magnetic brain responses during speech perception. Brain and Language, 149, 1-12. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.06.005

Tiemann, S., Kirsten, M., Beck, S., Hertrich, I., & Rolke, B. (2015). Presupposition processing and accommodation: An experiment on wieder (‘again’) and consequences for other triggers. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental Perspectives on Presuppositions: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics (pp. 39-65). Heidelberg: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-07980-6_2

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2014). Evidence for scene-based motion correspondence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 793-804. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0616-9

Rolke, B., Ruiz Fernández, S., Seibold, V. C., & Rahona, J. J. (2014). Crossed hands stay on the time-line. Cognitive Processing, 15(Suppl 1), 134-135.doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0632-2

Seibold V. C., Festl, F., & Rolke, B. (2014). Temporal preparation increases response conflict by faster direct response activation. Cognitive Processing, 15(Suppl 1), 67-68. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0632-2

Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B. (2014). Does temporal preparation facilitate visual processing in a selective manner? Evidence from attentional capture. Acta Psychologica, 137, 51-61. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.012

Kirsten, M., Tiemann, S., Seibold, V. C., Hertrich, I. Beck, S., & Rolke, B. (2014). When the polar bear encounters many polar bears: ERP context effects evoked by uniqueness failure. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 29, 1147-1162. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.899378

Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B. (2014). Does temporal preparation speed up visual processing? Evidence from the N2pc. Psychophysiology, 51, 529-538. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12196

Rahona, J. J., Ruiz Fernández, S., Rolke, B., Vázquez, C., & Hervás, G. (2014). Overt head movements moderate the effect of depressive symptoms on mood regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1328-1337. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.881323

Rolke, B., Ruiz Fernández, S., Schmid, M., Walker, M., Lachmair, M., Rahona López, J. J., Hervás, G., & Vázquez, C. (2013). Priming the mental time-line: Effects of modality and processing mode. Cognitive Processing, 14, 231-244. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0537-5

Ruiz Fernández, S., Leonhard, T., Lachmaier, M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Processing order in dual-tasks when the duration of motor responses varies. Universitas Psychologica, 12(5), 442-449. doi:10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-5.podt

Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Reply to: Testing of `Executive Function´. Chronobiology International, 29, 1285. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.728412

Rolke, B. (2012). Der Ausbruch des Gedanken - der Mensch als Randwesen der Evolution (The outpouring of thoughts: Humanity on the margins of evolution). In M. Flacke (Ed.), Verführung Freiheit: Kunst in Europa seit 1945, XXX. Europaratsausstellung (pp. 284-307). Berlin: Deutsches Historisches Museum.

Bratzke, D., Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2012). Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and the Simon task. Chronobiology International, 29, 55-61. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.635235

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2012). Spatio-temporal priority revisited: The role of feature identity and similarity for object correspondence in apparent motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 975-988. doi: 10.1037/a0028197

Hein, E., & Cavanagh, P. (2012). Motion correspondence shows feature bias in spatiotopic coordinates. Journal of Vision, 12(7), 16. doi: 10.1167/12.7.16, http://journalofvision.org/12/7/16/

Festl, F., Recktenwald, F., Yuan, C., & Mallot, H. A. (2012). Detection of linear ego-acceleration from optic flow. Journal of Vision, 12(7), 10. doi: 10.1167/12.7.10, http://journalofvision.org/12/7/10

Tiemann, S., Schmid, M., Bade, N., Rolke, B., Hertrich, I., Ackermann, H., Knapp, J., & Beck, S. (2011). Psycholinguistic evidence for presuppositions: On-line and off-line data. In I. Reich, E. Horch, & D. Pauly (Eds.), Proceedings of Sinn & Bedeutung 15 (pp. 581-595). Saarbrücken: Universaar – Saarland Unversity Press.

Ruiz-Fernandez, S., Leonhard, T., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Processing two task with varying task order: Central stage duration influences central processing order. Acta Psychologica, 137, 10-17. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.016

Seibold, V. C., Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Does temporal preparation increase the rate of sensory information accumulation? Acta Psychologica, 137, 56-64. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.02.006.

Heil, M., Kavsek, M., Rolke, B., Beste, C., & Jansen, P. (2011). Mental rotation in female fraternal twins: Evidence for intra-uterine hormone transfer? Biological Psychology, 86, 90-93. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.11.002

Seibold, V. C., Fiedler, A., & Rolke, B. (2011). Temporal attention shortens perceptual latency: A temporal prior entry effect. Psychophysiology, 48, 708-717. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01135.x

Steinborn, M. B., & Langner, R. (2011). Distraction by irrelevant sound during foreperiods selectively impairs temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 136, 405-418. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.008

Yeshurun, Y., & Hein, E. (2011). Transient attention degrades perceived apparent motion. Perception, 40, 905-918. doi: 10.1068/p7016

Fiedler, A., Schröter, H., Seibold, V. C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). The auditory redundant signals effect depends on the number of percepts: Evidence from the mismatch negativity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 68-84. doi: 10.3758/s13415-010-0013-y

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., Seibold, V. C., & Ulrich, R. (2010). Temporal preparation influences the dynamics of information processing: Evidence for early onset of information accumulation. Vision Research, 50, 1025-1034. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.011

Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2010). On the locus of temporal preparation: Enhancement of pre-motor processes. In A. C. Nobre & J. T. Coull (Eds.), Attention and time (pp. 227-241). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Seifried, T., Ulrich, R., Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Osman, A. (2010). Temporal preparation decreases perceptual latency: Evidence from a clock paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2431-2451. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.485354

Steinborn, M. B., Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Gordijn, M. C. M., Beersma, D. G. M., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of 40 hours of constant wakefulness on number comparison performance. Chronobiology International, 27, 807-825. doi: 10.3109/07420521003778765

Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 134, 94-104. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.011

Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Steinborn, M. B., & Ulrich, R. (2009). The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task-switching efficiency. Journal of Sleep Research, 18, 167-172. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00729.x

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2010). Investigating temporal properties of covert shifts of visual attention using the attentional walk task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 41-46. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.1.41

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2010). Unmasking the standing wave of invisibility: An account in terms of apparent motion and representational updating. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 398-408. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.2.398

Moore, C. M., Stephens, T. M., & Hein, E. (2010). Features, as well as space and time guide object persistence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 731-736. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.5.731

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2010). Lateral masking in cycling displays: the relative importance of separation, flanker duration and inter-stimulus interval. Perception, 39, 1330-1340. doi: 10.1068/p6643

Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2009). The source of execution-related dual-task interference: Motor bottleneck or response monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1413-1426. doi: 10.1037/a0015874

Hackley, S. A., Langner, R., Rolke, B., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Separation of phasic arousal and expectancy effects in a speeded reaction time task via fMRI. Psychophysiology, 46, 163-171. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00722.x

Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rolke, B. (2009). On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 273-310. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.08.003

Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132, 40-47. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.002

Moore, C. M., Hein, E., Grosjean, M., & Rinkenauer, G. (2009). Limited influence of perceptual organization on the precision of attentional control. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 971-983. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.4.971

Hein, E., & Moore, C. M. (2009). Explicit eye movements failed to facilitate the precision of later attentional localization. Experimental Brain Research, 194, 387-394. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1927-x

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Temporal preparation improves temporal resolution: Evidence from constant foreperiods. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1504-1514. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.8.1504

Bratzke, D., Ulrich, R., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., Jentzsch, I., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1385-1399. doi: 10.1080/17470210701536856

Rolke, B. (2008). Temporal preparation facilitates perceptual identification of letters. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1305-1313. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.7.1305

Rolke, B., Dinkelbach, A., Hein, E., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Does attention impair temporal discrimination? Examining non-attentional accounts. Psychological Research, 72, 49-60. doi: 10.1007/s00426-006-0092-0

Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129, 297-307. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.08.005

Rolke, B., & Hofmann, P. (2007). Temporal uncertainty degrades perceptual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 522-526. doi: 10.3758/BF03194101

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Knowing when to hear aids what to hear. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1610-1615. doi: 0.1080/17470210701536419

Bratzke, D., Rolke, B., Ulrich, R., & Peters, M. (2007). Central slowing during the night. Psychological Science, 18, 456-461. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01921.x

Rolke, B., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2007). Impaired temporal discrimination within the attentional blink. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1295-1304. doi: 10.3758/BF03192946

Hein, E., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2006). Visual attention and temporal discrimination: Differential effects of automatic and voluntary cueing. Visual Cognition, 13, 29-50. doi: 10.1080/13506280500143524

Rolke, B., Ulrich, R., & Bausenhart, K. M. (2006). Attention delays perceived stimulus offset. Vision Research, 46, 2926-2933. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.02.022

Ulrich, R., Fernandez, S. R., Jentzsch, I., Rolke, B., Schröter, H., & Leuthold, H. (2006). Motor limitation in dual-task processing under ballistic movement conditions. Psychological Science, 17, 788-793. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01783.x

Bausenhart, K. M., Rolke, B., Hackley, S. A., & Ulrich, R. (2006). The locus of temporal preparation effects: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 536-542. doi: 10.3758/BF03193882

Rolke, B. (2003). Ein Wort und seine Folgen. Gehirn & Geist, 3, 73-75.

Heil, M., & Rolke, B. (2004). Unattended distractor-induced priming in a visual selective attention task - N400 effects in the absence of RT effects. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 164-169. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.18.4.164

Heil, M., Rolke, B., & Pecchinenda, A. (2004). Automatic semantic activation is no myth - Semantic context effects on the N400 in the letter-search task in the absence of response time effects. Psychological Science, 15, 852-857. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00766.x

Niedeggen, M., Heil, M., Ludowig, E., Rolke, B., & Harris, C. L. (2004). Priming illusory words: an ERP approach. Neuropsychologia, 42, 745-753. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.007

Heil, M., Rosler, F., & Rolke, B. (2003). Another artificial division - and the data don't support it. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 739-740. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0333016X

Heil, M., & Rolke, B. (2002). Toward a chronopsychophysiology of mental rotation. Psychophysiology, 39, 414-422. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3940414

Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., & Hennighausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38, 165-174. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3820165

Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Hennighausen, E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: Inhibitory executive processes? Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 218-225. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.14.4.218

Rolke, B., Heil, M., Hennighausen, E., Haussler, C., & Rösler, F. (2000). Topography of brain electrical activity dissociates the sequential order transformation of verbal versus spatial information in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 282, 81-84. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00891-0

Heil, M., Rolke, B., Engelkamp, J., Rosler, F., Ozcan, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). Event-related brain potentials during recognition of ordinary and bizarre action phrases following verbal and subject-performed encoding conditions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 261-280. doi: 10.1080/713752313

Privacy settings

Our website uses cookies. Some of them are mandatory, while others allow us to improve your user experience on our website. The settings you have made can be edited at any time.

or

Essential

in2code

Videos

in2code
YouTube
Google